“The structures
which memory stores are not actual little pictures, but quasi-pictures, “representations” in the sense that the information
stored cause a change in the brain that encodes or molds it in a certain way
and in a particular “place” in the brain. […] What is involved in remembering
is the association and recollection of previously impressed material when the
original is no longer present to us. If the object should become actually
present to our senses again and we can compare our mental image to it for
accuracy, we are engaged in a process of recognizing
rather than of remembering or recollecting.” – Models for the Memory (chapter 1)
“Superimpositional storage: memories are
blended, not laid down independently once and for all, and are reconstructed
rather than reproduced. […] By ‘superimpositional storage’ I mean the property
that one network of units and connections may be used to store a number of
representations, so long as they are sufficiently distinct to coexist without
confusion.’” – John Sutton
Before I talk
about superimposition, I would like
to contrast the process of recognizing
and remembering/recollecting. The
first excerpt above explains to us that every new character we take into memory
will affect all previously impressed material of similar character: in the
sense that if one is to recollect in his or her head what a chair looks like,
he or she will not remember a specific chair – unless it is by choice, and only
possible if this specific chair is particularly distinct to all other chairs
the person has encountered – but an ideal version of a chair influenced by all
the various “quasi-pictures” of chairs already embedded in memory. Recollecting would be to conjure up an
image of a chair without having actually present, while recognizing would be to see a chair and to recognize it is a chair
when compared to all the images of the chairs (or a collective ideal image of a
chair) in the mind.
The second excerpt is my
interpretation to the first part of the first excerpt. Superimposition is the way our memories try to remember and
distinguish all the different shapes and sizes (etc.) of chairs someone has
encountered in real life. Although, since the brain’s memory function works via
connectivity of associations, the ability to distinguish between chairs is
dependent upon how orthogonal one chair is to another – for two sets of chairs that
are similar within the brain, it is most likely that it will be harder for the
brain to distinguish between the two.
In other words, superimpositional storage
plays a huge role in recollection and recognition in that it interprets new
memory traces not by its individual characteristics but with everything and
anything that is a memory trace in the brain. The more similar the new trace is
to any other traces already in the brain, the harder it will be to distinguish
because it is presumed that the more similar the memory traces are, the higher
chance that they would be processed within the same (or closely interwoven)
network of units and connections (groups of neural linkages), potentially
leading to distortion or error when the memory is needed to be tapped.
Some would say that superimpositional storage has a major downside in that every memory is practically
distorted the moment one recollects something for the first time, while I would
argue that this is probably the reason the brain can store a practically infinite
amount of memory while never running out of “disk space” like a computer would.
No comments:
Post a Comment